Commentary
4.1.1
MOTIVATION
Was I right? Try this trick on a few of your colleagues - it works especially
well if you already have the word written on a flashcard that you can
pull out after they have answered. I've tried this many times, including
in classes of teachers on training courses. I reckon that at least 90%
have the same answer.
Commentary
4.1.2
Motivation is often defined quite simply as something like 'a driving
force', but Williams and Burden (1997:120) give us a more complete definition:
Motivation may be construed as:
- a state of cognitive and
emotional arousal
- which leads to a conscious
decision to act and
- which gives rise to a period
of sustained intellectual and/or physical effort
- in order to attain a previously
set goal (or goals).
Commentary
4.1.3
- A
syllabus
Most teachers are expected to work from one, but the fact that many
learners appear not to learn everything on the syllabus, but do learn
things that aren't on it, calls the importance of syllabuses
into question. And of course, there are all those people who pick up
a language with no formal syllabus at all. So this is definitely not
essential. Some kind of syllabus may be desirable (or indeed, considered
necessary by administrators) in order to monitor student progress, make
clear the intended learning outcomes, etc, of a course, but these reasons
should not be confused with the how a syllabus relates to what learners
actually learn, and whether they can learn without a pre-specified syllabus
(which they clearly can). (See also the comments about interlanguage
in the notes for 'Someone to correct your errors'.)
- Someone to correct your
errors
Many learners ask teachers to correct all their mistakes, especially
in writing, and may even believe the teacher is not doing his or her
job properly if he / she does not. Although reformulation as
a method of error correction may function as meaningful input,
most correction has no long-term effect. This is because each learner's
interlanguage (their 'imperfect' language system that is their
current approximation to the target language and the source of their
errors) develops in a unique and organic way. A particular form is not
acquired for accurate production purposes until the internal system
is ready, and it is impossible for anyone (including the learner) to
know when this is. Trying to force learning of a form, either through
error correction or through presentation and explicit teaching of the
form, is now widely considered to be futile except in the short term
for the purposes of traditional grammar tests. Instead, each learner
unconsciously 'constructs' his or her own syllabus as he or she progresses.
In this sense, a syllabus is more like an unwritten, retrospective or
ongoing personal progress record than a pre-prepared, externally constructed
list of items for future learning.
(See also the comments about syllabus in the notes for 'Syllabus'.)
- An extrovert personality
Personality may be a factor in that extroverts may be prepared to take
more risks and therefore engage in more pushed output. However,
we would do well to remember that everyone - extrovert and introvert
alike - learned their first language equally successfully, even if they
are normally heard to be using it in different quantities!
- Exposure to meaningful
input
This is indeed essential. Second language acquisition theory and research
findings (and common sense!) tell us that unless you can hear or read
samples of the target language, there is no way you could acquire any
of it. Furthermore, the input must be comprehensible (Krashen,
1981), that is, you must be able to understand at least in part what
you hear or read. If the language is meaningful in more than just a
superficial sense, perhaps because you relate to it in some way, because
it is a familiar topic or something you are interested in, then you
are likely to struggle harder to extract meaning, and your chances of
learning increase (in motivational terms, your state of arousal will
be higher and you will be prepared to make a greater effort to achieve
your goal of understanding). Task-based learning includes plenty of
meaningful input. But instead of making language meaningful by explaining
it before learners do a task, the learners try the task (so they
have a very clear idea of what they mean, even if they don't have the
language to express their meaning well), and then listen to fluent speakers
doing the task. In this way, even quite complex input is rendered highly
meaningful to the learners.
- Information
about grammar rules
Some information, if it is relevant to the current communicative needs
of the learner, may be helpful, and learners are often reassured by
it since this is what most learners have come to equate with classroom
language learning. However, the systematic transmission of rules and
other grammatical information that was typical of traditional language
teaching is almost certainly not very helpful if the aim is to enable
communicative competence. Conscious knowledge about language
does not necessarily lead to competence in language.
(See also the notes for 'Instruction from a teacher'.)
- A coursebook
A coursebook can provide useful input and suggest activities that help
language acquisition (eg by encouraging pushed output). But clearly
a coursebook is not essential. At worst, such a book may focus exclusively
on explicit teaching of grammar (see the notes for 'Instruction from
a teacher' and 'Information about grammar rules') and have as its goal
grammatical accuracy, being based on the mistaken belief that input
equals intake. Such books are based on the behaviourist theory that
practice and the quest for accuracy (eg drills and grammar exercises)
will result in communicative competence. Beware!
- A high IQ
People with high IQs might get to grips with grammar rules faster than
others, but such knowledge about language does not necessarily
lead to communicative competence (see the notes for 'Information about
grammar rules'), People of all intellectual abilities learn languages
successfully under the right circumstances.
- Using the language
This too is essential. SLA research findings suggest that 'pushed
output', or struggling to communicate something beyond your current
capability, helps you to use the language you know creatively and gives
you both motivation to learn and a clear idea of what it is you need
to learn. The central premise of task-based learning is that learners
should be encouraged to use whatever language they have in order to
complete a task, before moving on to consider what language they
lack to do this as well as they might. While doing the task, they are
free to simply focus on getting their message across as best they can,
with no prior recommendations from teacher or book on what forms to
use. In TBL, the task stage is not just a practice, 'free' or
'production' stage.
- Starting when very young
It would seem that children and adults learn in different ways (adults
use more cognitive strategies), and some children do seem to learn a
second language almost as effortlessly as their first. This usually
happens when they are immersed in the language at school, for example.
As teachers, however, there's not much we can do about the age of our
learners, and in further and higher education we can safely assume we
are dealing with adults - who can and do learn successfully
too!
- Instruction
from a teacher
Instruction - or being taught - is not essential. Millions of people
'pick up' additional languages without the help of a teacher (or instructional
book), some to very high levels of proficiency. However, recent research
(see Nunan, 1999: 42-50 for a summary) suggests that instruction which
focuses on language form can speed up the learning process and helps
to prevent early fossilization. This is an important consideration where
learners are acquiring a language mainly in a classroom setting and
where time is normally very limited. Activity cycle 5 of this module
(see 4.5 suggests how instruction can be incorporated
into a task-based lesson for maximum effect.
(See also the notes for 'Information about grammar
rules'.)
Commentary
4.1.4
So far we have identified three essential conditions for language
acquisition: these are: motivation, meaningful input (or exposure)
and pushed output.
Commentary
4.1.5
In addition, there is one other factor that is especially desirable. This
is: instruction.
Commentary
4.1.6
use; motivation; input; instruction; exposure; form.
|